‘I'll Wear This as a Badge of Honour Because It's Made Me Better’: Patient and Caregiver Narrative Accounts of DBS for Depression
By Cassandra Thomson
This piece is part of a series of featured posts from the 2020 International Neuroethics Society Meeting. It is based on an abstract of the same title that won the award for the “Best Abstract.”
![]() |
Image courtesy of Pixabay |
One aspect of DBS clinical trials for depression that has been queried is the outcome measures used to determine the intervention’s success. Primary outcome measures often used to establish treatment response are the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression and Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale. Both assess depression symptom severity, frequency and intensity, but some question whether these alone are adequate to comprehensively gauge the impact of DBS – particularly in individuals with such severe and refractory depression (Fins et al., 2017). What ‘well’ looks like and what is considered a ‘success’ is highly specific to the individual (Mayberg, 2018). Supplementing standard psychometric measures with narrative accounts can provide richer insight into the personal significance and meaning of outcomes and help develop more person-centred approaches to clinical care. Another aspect absent from DBS depression studies to-date is the perspective of caregivers. Partners and family members of DBS participants can be directly impacted by the trial commitments and outcomes (Thomson, Segrave, Gardner, & Carter, 2019). For this reason, our research explored the lived experiences of both DBS patients and caregivers, who are vital partners in the recovery process (Thomson, Segrave, Fitzgerald, et al., 2020).
Within neuroethics, a common point of debate has been: does DBS alter an individual’s personality, sense of self, and identity? (Gilbert, Viaña, & Ineichen, 2018). In other DBS indications, including Parkinson’s disease and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), qualitative studies have revealed important insights into how the intervention impacts the individual’s perceptions of themselves, their bodies, and their relationships. In Parkinson’s disease, patients and caregivers have reported both positive and negative personality changes emerging after DBS (e.g., more fun, open, talkative; more aggressive, selfish, quiet) (Lewis et al., 2015). When motor symptoms are substantially improved, some feel they can now express their true personality and that their old self has been restored (Thomson, Segrave, Racine, et al., 2020). Conversely, some patients have reported difficulty accepting the implanted electrical device psychologically and have experienced altered body image, while caregivers have reported feeling “lost” now that their partner no longer depends on them (Schüpbach et al., 2006). When unintended side-effects emerge (e.g., irritability, compulsive behaviours), spouses have reported feeling as though they are married to a different person (Mosley et al., 2019). In OCD, patients have described post-DBS changes as being more or less aligned with their perceived true self (de Haan, Rietveld, Stokhof, & Denys, 2017). Some expressed needing to get used to how they are now or needing to find out who they are without OCD. These studies have added much to the ongoing debate within neuroethics around how DBS impacts personality, identity and self; however, these concepts are yet to be examined in those with treatment-resistant depression.
![]() |
Image courtesy of Wikimedia Commons |
Such findings provide a holistic understanding of the psychological impact of what is often considered a biological treatment. The narrative accounts allow personally significant benefits, as well as unexpected challenges, to be appreciated. It is important, however, to recognise the limited generalisability of such findings. Qualitative results must be considered in the specific context from which the data is derived (e.g., geographical location, sample demographics, implantation site, research protocol). Remaining mindful of this and not overgeneralising qualitative findings from small samples reduces the risk of a neuroethics bubble developing.
References
- de Haan, S., Rietveld, E., Stokhof, M., & Denys, D. (2017). Becoming more oneself? Changes in personality following DBS treatment for psychiatric disorders: Experiences of OCD patients and general considerations. PLoS One, 12(4), e0175748. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0175748
- Dobbs, D. (2018, 17/04/2018). Why a ‘lifesaving’ depression treatment didn’t pass clinical trials…but could still be a groundbreaking therapy.
- Dougherty, D. D., Rezai, A. R., Carpenter, L. L., Howland, R. H., Bhati, M. T., O'Reardon, J. P., . . . Malone, D. A., Jr. (2015). A Randomized Sham-Controlled Trial of Deep Brain Stimulation of the Ventral Capsule/Ventral Striatum for Chronic Treatment-Resistant Depression. Biol Psychiatry, 78(4), 240-248. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.023
- Fava, M. (2003). Diagnosis and definition of treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry, 53, 649-659.
- Fins, J. J., Kubu, C. S., Mayberg, H. S., Merkel, R., Nuttin, B., & Schlaepfer, T. E. (2017). Being open minded about neuromodulation trials: Finding success in our "failures". Brain Stimul, 10(2), 181-186. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2016.12.012
- Fitzgerald, P. B. (2016). Deep brain stimulation in depression. Aust N Z J Psychiatry, 50(1), 94-97. doi:10.1177/0004867415611755
- Fitzgerald, P. B., & Segrave, R. A. (2015). Deep brain stimulation in mental health: Review of evidence for clinical efficacy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49(11), 979-993. doi:10.1177/0004867415598011
- Frieden, T. R. (2017). Evidence for Health Decision Making - Beyond Randomized, Controlled Trials. New England Journal of Medicine, 377(5), 465-475. doi:10.1056/NEJMra1614394
- Gilbert, F., Viaña, J. N. M., & Ineichen, C. (2018). Deflating the “DBS causes personality changes” bubble. Neuroethics. doi:10.1007/s12152-018-9373-8
- Holtzheimer, P. E., Husain, M. M., Lisanby, S. H., Taylor, S. F., Whitworth, L. A., McClintock, S., . . . Mayberg, H. S. (2017). Subcallosal cingulate deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression: a multisite, randomised, sham-controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(11), 839-849. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(17)30371-1
- James, S. L., Abate, D., Abate, K. H., Abay, S. M., Abbafati, C., Abbasi, N., . . . Murray, C. J. L. (2018). Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. The Lancet, 392(10159), 1789-1858. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(18)32279-7
- Lewis, C. J., Maier, F., Horstkotter, N., Zywczok, A., Witt, K., Eggers, C., & Meyer et al., T. D. (2015). Subjectively perceived personality and mood changes associated with subthalamic stimulation in patients with Parkinson's disease. Psychological Medicine, 45(1), 73-85. doi:10.1017/S0033291714001081
- Malone, D. A., Jr., Dougherty, D. D., Rezai, A. R., Carpenter, L. L., Friehs, G. M., Eskandar, E. N., . . . Greenberg, B. D. (2009). Deep brain stimulation of the ventral capsule/ventral striatum for treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry, 65(4), 267-275. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.029
- Mayberg, H. S. (2018, July 2018). What is well? Reconciling First- and Third- Person Perspectives on Depression Recovery with DBS. Paper presented at the Federation of European Neuroscience Societies 11th FENS Forum of Neuroscience, Berlin, Germany.
- Mayberg, H. S., Lozano, A. M., Voon, V., McNeely, H. E., Seminowicz, D., Hamani, C., . . . Kennedy, S. H. (2005). Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron, 45(5), 651-660. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.014
- Mosley, P. E., Robinson, K., Coyne, T., Silburn, P., Breakspear, M., & Carter, A. (2019). ‘Woe Betides Anybody Who Tries to Turn me Down.’ A Qualitative Analysis of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms Following Subthalamic Deep Brain Stimulation for Parkinson’s Disease. Neuroethics. doi:10.1007/s12152-019-09410-x
- Schlaepfer, T. E. (2015). Deep Brain Stimulation for Major Depression—Steps on a Long and Winding Road. Biological Psychiatry, 78(4), 218-219. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.020
- Schüpbach, W. M., Gargiulo, M., Welter, M. L., Mallet, L., Behar, C., Houeto, J. L., . . . Agid, Y. (2006). Neurosurgery in Parkinson disease: a distressed mind in a repaired body? Neurology, 66(12), 1811-1816. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000234880.51322.16
- Thomson, C., Segrave, R., Gardner, J., & Carter, A. (2019). Patients’ Weighing of the Long-Term Risks and Consequences Associated With Deep Brain Stimulation in Treatment-Resistant Depression. AJOB Neuroscience, 9(4), 243-245. doi:10.1080/21507740.2018.1561542
- Thomson, C. J., Segrave, R. A., Fitzgerald, P. B., Richardson, K. E., Racine, E., & Carter, A. (2020, October 2020). ‘I’ll wear this as a badge of honour because it’s made me better’: Illness, self-concept and device embodiment in DBS for treatment-resistant depression. Paper presented at the International Neuroethics Society Annual Meeting, Virtual conference.
- Thomson, C. J., Segrave, R. A., Racine, E., Warren, N., Thyagarajan, D., & Carter, A. (2020). “He’s Back so I’m Not Alone”: The Impact of Deep Brain Stimulation on Personality, Self, and Relationships in Parkinson’s Disease. Qualitative Health Research, 30(14), 2217-2233. doi:10.1177/1049732320951144
______________